Chapter published in:
Bilingualism, Executive Function, and Beyond: Questions and insights
Edited by Irina A. Sekerina, Lauren Spradlin and Virginia Valian
[Studies in Bilingualism 57] 2019
► pp. 161180
References

References

Abutalebi, J., Green, D. W.
(2008) Control mechanisms in bilingual language production: Neural evidence from language switching studies. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23, 557–582. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Backus, A.
(2015) A usage-based approach to codeswitching: The need for reconciling structure and function. In: G. Stell, K. Yapko eds. (Eds). Code-switching at the crossroads between structural and sociolinguistic perspectives. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Bhatt, R., Bolonyai, A.
(2011) Code-switching and the optimal grammar of bilingual language use. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14, 522–546. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E.
(2009) Bilingualism: The good, the bad, and the indifferent. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12, 3–11. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2010) Global-Local and Trail-Making Tasks by Monolingual and Bilingual children: Beyond Inhibition. Developmental Psychology, 46, 93–105. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. G., Luk, G.
(2012) Bilingualism: Consequences for Mind and Brain. Trends in Cognitive Science, 16, 240–250. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Botvinick, M., Carter, C. S., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Cohen, J. D.
(2001) Conflict-monitoring and Cognitive Control. Psychological Review, 108, 624–652. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Clyne, M.
(2003) Dynamics of Language Change. Cambridge Approaches to Language Contact, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Costa, A., Hernandez, M., Sebastian-Galles, N.
(2008) Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition, 106, 59–86. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Hernandez, M., Costa-Faidella, J., Sebastian-Galles, N.
(2009) On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition, 113, 135–149. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
De Groot, A. M. B., Christoffels, I. K.
(2006) Language Control in bilinguals: Monolingual tasks and simultaneous interpreting. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 9, 189–201. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Deuchar, M., Muysken, P. & Wang, S.
(2008) Structured Variation in Codeswitching: Towards an Empirically Based Typology of Bilingual Speech Patterns. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 10, 298–34. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Eppler, E.
(2005) The Syntax of German-English code-switching. PhD Thesis, UCL.Google Scholar
Gardner-Chloros, P.
(2009) Code-switching. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Green, D. W.
(1998) Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 67–81. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Green, D. W., Abutalebi, J.
(2013) Language Control in Bilinguals: The Adaptive Control Hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 515–530. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Green, D. W., Wei, L.
(2014) A control process model of CS. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 24, 499–511. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Guo, T., Liu, H., Misra, M., Kroll, J. F.
(2011) Local and global inhibition in bilingual word production: fMRI evidence from Chinese-English bilinguals. Neuroimage, 56, 2300–2309. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Li, P., Zhang, F., Tsai, E., Puls, B.
(2013) Language history questionnaire (LHQ 2.0): A new dynamic web-based research tool. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17, 673–680. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Morales, J., Yudes, C., Gomez-Ariza, C. J., Teresa Bajo, M.
(2015) Bilingualism modulates dual mechanisms of cognitive control: Evidence from ERPs. Neuropsychologia, 66, 157–169. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Muysken, P.
(2000) Bilingual Speech: A Typology of Code-mixing. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Paap, K. R., Greenberg, Z. I.
(2013) There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing, Cognitive Psychology, 66, 232–258. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Prior, A., Gollan, T. H.
(2011) Good language-switchers are good task-switchers: evidence from Spanish-English and Mandarin-English bilinguals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17, 682–691. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H.
(1998) Manual for Raven’s progressive matrices and vocabulary scales. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Soveri, A., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., Laine, M.
(2011) Is there a relationship between language switching and executive functions in bilingualism? Introducing a within-group analysis approach. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, Article 183.Google Scholar
Treffers-Daller, J.
(1998) The IC model and code-switching. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 98–99.Google Scholar
(2009) Code-switching and transfer: an exploration of similarities and differences. In: Bullock, B. E. and Almeida, J. T. (Eds). The Cambridge Handbook of Linguistic Codeswitching. Cambridge University Press, pp. 58–74. CrossrefGoogle Scholar