Article published in:
Interaction and Iconicity in the Evolution of Language
Edited by Stefan Hartmann, Michael Pleyer, James Winters and Jordan Zlatev
[Interaction Studies 18:3] 2017
► pp. 314329
References

References

Arbib, M. A.
(2005) From monkey-like action recognition to human language: An evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(02), 105–124. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Arbib, M. A., Liebal, K., & Pika, S.
(2008) Primate vocalization, gesture, and the evolution of human language. Current Anthropology, 49(6), 1053–1076. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Atkinson, Q. D., Meade, A., Venditti, C., Greenhill, S. J., & Pagel, M.
(2008) Languages evolve in punctuational bursts. Science, 319(5863), 588. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baus, C., Carreiras, M., & Emmorey, K.
(2013) When does iconicity in sign language matter? Language and Cognitive Processes, 28(3), 261–271. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bavelas, J. B., Coates, L., & Johnson, T.
(2000) Listeners as co-narrators. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(6), 941–952. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bellugi, U., & Klima, E. S.
(1976) Two faces of sign: Iconic and abstract. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 280(1), 514–538. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bohn, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M.
(2016) Comprehension of iconic gestures by chimpanzees and human children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 142(C), 1–17. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Brennan, S. E., & Clark, H. H.
(1996) Conceptual pacts and lexical choice in conversation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 22(6), 1482–1493.Google Scholar
Cangelosi, A., & Parisi, D.
(2002) Computer simulation: A new scientific approach to the study of language evolution. In Simulating the evolution of language (pp. 3–28). London: Springer London. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H., & Brennan, S. E.
(1991) Grounding in communication. Perspectives on Socially Shared Cognition, 13, 127–149. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H., & Wilkes-Gibbs, D.
(1986) Referring as a collaborative process. Cognition, 22(1), 1–39. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Deacon, T. W.
(1998) The symbolic species: The co-evolution of language and the brain. New York & London: W. W. Norton & Company.Google Scholar
Dediu, D., & Levinson, S. C.
(2013) On the antiquity of language: The reinterpretation of Neandertal linguistic capacities and its consequences. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 397. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fay, N., & Ellison, T. M.
(2013) The cultural evolution of human communication systems in different sized populations: Usability trumps learnability. PLoS ONE, 8(8), e71781–9. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fay, N., Ellison, T. M., & Garrod, S.
(2014) Iconicity: From sign to system in human communication and language. Pragmatics & Cognition, 22(2), 244–263. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fay, N., Arbib, M., & Garrod, S.
(2013) How to bootstrap a human communication system. Cognitive Science, 37(7), 1356–1367. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fay, N., Garrod, S., & Roberts, L.
(2008) The fitness and functionality of culturally evolved communication systems. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 363(1509), 3553–3561. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fay, N., Garrod, S., Roberts, L., & Swoboda, N.
(2010) The interactive evolution of human communication systems. Cognitive Science, 34(3), 351–386. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fay, N., Lister, C. J., Ellison, T. M., & Goldin-Meadow, S.
(2014) Creating a communication system from scratch: Gesture beats vocalization hands down. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–12. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fay, N., Walker, B., Swoboda, N., & Garrod, S.
(2016) How to create a shared symbol system: Observation, cognitive bias and social interaction. Manuscript under revision.
Fitch, W. T.
(2005) The evolution of language: A comparative review. Biology & Philosophy, 20(2–3), 193–203. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Frishberg, N.
(1975) Arbitrariness and iconicity: Historical change in American Sign Language. Language, 51(3), 696–719. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fusaroli, R., Bahrami, B., Olsen, K., Roepstorff, A., Rees, G., Frith, C., & Tylen, K.
(2012) Coming to terms: Quantifying the benefits of linguistic coordination. Psychological Science, 23(8), 931–939. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Galantucci, B.
(2005) An experimental study of the emergence of human communication systems. Cognitive Science, 29(5), 737–767. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Garrod, S., & Anderson, A.
(1987) Saying what you mean in dialogue: A study in conceptual and semantic co-ordination. Cognition, 27(2), 181–218. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Garrod, S., & Pickering, M. J.
(2004) Why is conversation so easy? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(1), 8–11. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Garrod, S., Fay, N., Lee, J., Oberlander, J., & MacLeod, T.
(2007) Foundations of representation: Where might graphical symbol systems come from? Cognitive Science, 31(6), 961–987. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Garrod, S., Fay, N., Rogers, S., Walker, B., & Swoboda, N.
(2010) Can iterated learning explain the emergence of graphical symbols? Interaction Studies, 11(1), 33–50. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gasser, M.
(2004) The origins of arbitrariness in language. Presented at the Proceedings of the 26th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 434–439). Mahwah, NJ.
Goldin-Meadow, S.
(2010) Widening the lens on language learning: Language creation in deaf children and adults in Nicaragua. Human Development, 53(5), 303–311. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Harnad, S.
(1990) The symbol grounding problem. Physica D: Nonlinear Phenomena, 42(1–3), 335–346. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Healey, P. G. T., Swoboda, N., Umata, I., & Katagiri, Y.
(2002) Graphical representation in graphical dialogue. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 57(4), 375–395. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hockett, C. F.
(1978) In search of Jove's Brow. American Speech, 53(4), 243–313. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hupet, M., & Chantraine, Y.
(1992) Changes in repeated references: Collaboration or repetition effects? Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 21(6), 485–496. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hurford, J. R.
(2007) The origins of meaning (Vol. 1 of Languge in the light of evolution). Oxford: Oxford University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Imai, M., & Kita, S.
(2014) The sound symbolism bootstrapping hypothesis for language acquisition and language evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369(1651), 20130298. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kirby, S.
(2001) Spontaneous evolution of linguistic structure-an iterated learning model of the emergence of regularity and irregularity. IEEE Transactions on Evolutionary Computation, 5(2), 102–110. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lieberman, E., Michel, J.-B., Jackson, J., Tang, T., & Nowak, M. A.
(2007) Quantifying the evolutionary dynamics of language. Nature, 449(7163), 713–716. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lister, C. J., Fay, N., Ellison, T. M., & Ohan, J.
(2015) Creating a new communication system: Gesture has the upper hand. In Proceedings of the 37th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society .
Monaghan, P., Shillcock, R. C., Christiansen, M. H., & Kirby, S.
(2014) How arbitrary is language? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369(1651), 20130299. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Oudgenoeg-Paz, O., Volman, M. C. J. M., & Leseman, P. P. M.
(2012) Attainment of sitting and walking predicts development of productive vocabulary between ages 16 and 28 months. Infant Behavior and Development, 35(4), 733–736. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pagel, M., Atkinson, Q. D., Calude, A. S., & Meade, A.
(2013) Ultraconserved words point to deep language ancestry across Eurasia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(21), 8471–8476. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Perlman, M., & Cain, A. A.
(2016) Iconicity in vocalization, comparisons with gesture, and implications for theories on the evolution of language. Gesture, 14(3), 320–350. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Perlman, M., Dale, R., & Lupyan, G.
(2015) Iconicity can ground the creation of vocal symbols. Royal Society Open Science, 2(8), 150152–16. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Perniss, P., & Vigliocco, G.
(2014) The bridge of iconicity: From a world of experience to the experience of language. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369(1651), 20130300. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Perniss, P., Thompson, R. L., & Vigliocco, G.
(2010) Iconicity as a General Property of Language: Evidence from Spoken and Signed Languages. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 1–15. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Perry, L. K., Perlman, M., & Lupyan, G.
(2015) Iconicity in English and Spanish and its relation to lexical category and age of acquisition. PLoS ONE, 10(9), e0137147–17. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S.
(2004) Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(2), 169–190. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Peirce, C. S.
(1931–1958) Collected papers of Charles Sanders Peirce. Edited by C. Hathorne, P. Weiss, & A. Burks. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University PressGoogle Scholar
Roberts, G., Lewandowski, J., & Galantucci, B.
(2015) How communication changes when we cannot mime the world: Experimental evidence for the effect of iconicity on combinatoriality. Cognition, 141(C), 52–66. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rowe, M. L., & Goldin-Meadow, S.
(2009) Early gesture selectivelypredicts later language learning. Developmental Science, 12(1), 182–187. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sandler, W., Aronoff, M., Meir, I., & Padden, C.
(2011) The gradual emergence of phonological form in a new language. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 29(2), 503–543. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Saussure, F. de
1916A course in general linguistics. New York: Philosophical Library.Google Scholar
Schober, M. F., & Clark, H. H.
(1989) Understanding by addressees and overhearers. Cognitive Psychology, 21(2), 211–232. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Scott-Phillips, T. C., Kirby, S., & Ritchie, G. R. S.
(2009) Signalling signalhood and the emergence of communication. Cognition, 113(2), 226–233. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L., & Bergman, T. J.
(2005) Primate social cognition and the origins of language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(6), 264–266. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Steels, L.
(2011) Modeling the cultural evolution of language. Physics of Life Reviews, 8(4), 339–356. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, K., & Tallerman, M.
(2012) The Oxford handbook of language evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Theisen, C. A., Oberlander, J., & Kirby, S.
(2010) Systematicity and arbitrariness in novel communication systems. Interaction Studies, 11(1), 14–32. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wescott, R. W.
(1971) Linguistic iconism. Language, 47(2), 416–428. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wilkes-Gibbs, D., & Clark, H. H.
(1992) Coordinating beliefs in conversation. Journal of Memory and Language, 31(2), 183–194. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 2 other publications

Fay, Nicolas, Bradley Walker, Nik Swoboda, Ichiro Umata, Takugo Fukaya, Yasuhiro Katagiri & Simon Garrod
2018. Universal Principles of Human Communication: Preliminary Evidence From a Cross-cultural Communication Game. Cognitive Science 42:7  pp. 2397 ff. Crossref logo
Lister, C.J., B. Walker & N. Fay
2020. Innovation and enculturation in child communication: a cross-sectional study. Evolutionary Human Sciences 2 Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 06 march 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.