Article published in:
Gesture
Vol. 15:2 (2016) ► pp. 224249
References

References

Arnold, Kate & Klaus Zuberbühler
(2006a) The alarm-calling system of adult male putty-nosed monkeys (Cercopithecus nictitans martini). Animal Behaviour, 72, 643–653. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2006b) Language evolution: Semantic combinations in primate calls. Nature, 441, 303. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bard, Kim
(1992) Intentional behavior and intentional communication in young free-ranging orangutans. Child Development, 63, 1186–1197. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Boesch, Christophe
(1991a) Symbolic communication in wild chimpanzees? Human Evolution, 6 (1), 81–90. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1991b) Teaching among wild chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 41, 530–532. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1995) Innovation in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). International Journal of Primatology, 16 (1), 1–16. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1996) Three approaches for assessing chimpanzee culture. In Anne E. Russon, Kim A. Bard, & Sue T. Parker (Eds.), Reaching into thought: The minds of the great apes (pp. 404–429). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
(2003) Is culture a golden barrier between human and chimpanzee? Evolutionary Anthropology, 12, 82–91. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2012a) From material to symbolic cultures: Culture in primates. In Jaan Valsiner (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of culture and psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2012b) Wild cultures. A comparison between chimpanzee and human cultures. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Boesch, Christophe & Hedwige Boesch-Achermann
(1984) Mental map in wild chimpanzees: An analysis of hammer transports for nut cracking. Primates, 25, 160–170. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, Richard W.
(1993) A formal notation to aid analysis of complex behaviour – Understanding the tactical deception of primates. Behaviour, 127, 231–246. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2003) Tracing the evolutionary path of cognition: Tactical deception in primates. In Martin Brüne, Hedda Ribbert, & Wulf Schiefenhövel (Eds.), The social brain: Evolution and pathology (pp. 43–609). Chichester, England: John Wiley. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2010) Deception: Competition by misleading behavior. In Michael D. Breed & Janice Moore (Eds.), Encyclopaedia of animal behavior (Vol. 1, pp. 461–465). Oxford: Academic Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, Richard W. & Andrew Whiten
(1990) Tactical deception in primates: The 1990 database. Primate report, 27, 1101.Google Scholar
(1992) Cognitive evolution in primates: Evidence from Tactical Deception. Man, 27 (3), 609–627. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Call, Josep
(2011) How artificial communication affects the communication and cognition of the great apes. Mind & Language, 26, 1–20. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Call, Josep, Brian Agnetta, & Michael Tomasello
(2000) Cues that chimpanzees do and do not use to find hidden objects. Animal Cognition, 3, 23–34. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Call, Josep & Michael Tomasello
(2008) Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12 (5), 187–192. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cheney, Dorothy L. & Robert M. Seyfarth
(2007) Baboon metaphysics. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Crockford, Catherine & Christophe Boesch
(2003) Context-specific calls in wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus: analysis of barks. Animal Behaviour, 66, 115–125. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2005) Call combinations in wild chimpanzees. Behaviour, 142, 397–421. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Crockford, Catherine, Ilka Herbinger, Linda Vigilant, & Christophe Boesch
(2004) Wild chimpanzees produce group-specific calls: A case for vocal learning? Ethology, 110, 221–243. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Crockford, Catherine, Roman M. Wittig, Roger Mundry, & Klaus Zuberbühler
(2012) Wild chimpanzees inform ignorant group members of danger. Current Biology, 22, 142–146. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Deacon, Terrence W.
(1997) The symbolic species: The co-evolution of language and the brain. New York: W.W. Norton & Company.Google Scholar
Dennett, Daniel C.
(1987) The intentional stance. Cambridge, MA: MIT/Bradford Books.Google Scholar
Enfield, Nick J.
(2010) Human sociality at the heart of language [Inaugural lecture]. Nijmegen: Radboud University Nijmegen.Google Scholar
Fitch, W. Tecumseh
(2005) The evolution of language: A comparative review. Biology and Philosophy, 20, 193–230. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Genty, Emilie, Thomas Breuer, Catherine Hobaiter, & Richard W. Byrne
(2009) Gestural communication of the gorilla (Gorilla gorilla): Repertoire, intentionality and possible origins. Animal Cognition, 12, 527–546. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Goodall, Jane
(1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe, patterns of behavior. Cambridge, MA: The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Greenfield, Patricia M. & E. Sue Savage-Rumbaugh
(1990) Grammatical combination in Pan paniscus: Processes of learning and invention in the evolution and development of language. In Sue T. Parker & Kathleen R. Gibson (Eds.), ‘Language’ and intelligence in monkeys and apes: Comparative developmental perspectives (pp. 540–578). New York: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grice, Herbert P.
(1957) Meaning. The Philosophical Review, 66 (3), 377–388. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Griffin, Donald R.
(1984) Animal thinking. Cambridge, MA & London: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Hammerschmidt, Kurt & Julia Fischer
(2008) Constraints in primate vocal production. In Ulrike Griebel & D. Kimbrough Oller (Eds.), The evolution of communicative creativity: From fixed signals to contextual flexibility (pp. 93–119). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hare, Brian, Josep Call, Brian Agnetta, & Michael Tomasello
(2000) Chimpanzees know what conspecifics do and do not see. Animal Behaviour, 59, 771–785. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hare, Brian, Josep Call, & Michael Tomasello
(2001) Do chimpanzees know what conspecifics know? Animal Behaviour, 61, 139–151. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hauser, Marc D.
(1990) Do chimpanzee copulatory calls incite male–male competition? Animal Behaviour, 39, 596–597. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Heyes, Cecilia M.
(1994) Social learning in animals: Categories and mechanisms. Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society, 69, 207–231. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2012) What’s social about social learning? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126 (2), 193–202. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hobaiter, Catherine & Richard W. Byrne
(2011) The gestural repertoire of the wild chimpanzee. Animal Cognition, 14 (5), 745–767. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2014) The meanings of chimpanzee gestures. Current Biology, 24, 1596–1600. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hockett, Charles F.
(1960) The origin of speech. Scientific American, 203 (3), 89–96. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hoppitt, William J.E. & Kevin N. Laland
(2008) Social processes influencing learning in animals: A review of the evidence. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 38, 105–165. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2013) Social learning: An introduction to mechanisms, methods and models. Princeton: Princeton University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jaeggi, Adrain V., Maria A. van Noordwijk, & Carel P. van Schaik
(2008) Begging for information: mother–offspring food sharing among wild Bornean orangutans. American Journal of Primatology, 70, 533–541. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Janmaat, Karline R.L., Simone Dagui Ban, & Christophe Boesch
(2013) Chimpanzees use long-term spatial memory to monitor large fruit trees and remember feeding experiences across seasons. Animal Behaviour, 86 (6), 1183–1205. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kita, Sotaro
(2003) Pointing: A foundational building block of human communication. In Sotaro Kita (Ed.), Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet (pp. 93–119). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kummer, Hans
(1968) Social organization of Hamadryas baboons: A field study. (Bibliotheca Primatologica, 6). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
(1971) Primate societies: Group techniques of ecological adaptation. Chicago & New York: Aldine-Atherton.Google Scholar
Laland, Kevin N.
(2004) Social learning strategies. Learning & Behavior, 32 (1), 4–14. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Liebal, Katja, Simone Pika, & Michael Tomasello
(2006) Gestural communication of orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). Gesture, 6, 1–38. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Liebal, Katja & Josep Call
(2012) The origins of non-human primates’ manual gestures. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367 (1585), 118–128. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lyn, Heidi, Jamie L. Russell, David A. Leavens, Kim A. Bard, Sarah T. Boysen, Jennifer A. Schaeffer, & William D. Hopkins
(2014) Apes communicate about absent and displaced objects: methodology matters. Animal Cognition, 17 (1), 85–94. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
MacKinnon, John R.
(1974) The behaviour and ecology of wild orang-utans (Pongo pygmaeus). Animal Behaviour, 22, 3–74. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Menzel, Emil W. Jr
(1971) Communication about the environment in a group of young chimpanzees. Folia Primatologica, 15, 220–232. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Miles, H. Lyn
(1990) The cognitive foundations for reference in a signing orangutan. In Sue T. Parker & Kathleen R. Gibson (Eds.), Language and intelligence in monkeys and apes: Comparative developmental perspectives (pp. 511–539). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Milton, Katharine
(1981) Distribution patterns of tropical plant foods as an evolutionary stimulus to primate mental development. American Anthropologist, 83, 534–548. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Moore, Richard
forthcoming). Enacting and understanding communicative intent.
Mulcahy, Nicholas J. & Josep Call
(2006) Apes save tools for future use. Science, 312, 1038–1040. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, Toshisada
(1980) The leaf-clipping display: A newly-discovered expressive gesture in wild chimpanzees. Journal of Human Evolution, 9, 117–128. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1987) Local traditions and cultural transmission. In Barbara B. Smuts, Dorothy L. Cheney, Robert M. Seyfarth, Richard W. Wrangham, & Thomas T. Struhsaker (Eds.), Primate societies (pp. 462–474). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
(1997) Sexual behavior of adult male chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Primates, 38, 379–398. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Normand, Emanuelle, Simone Dagui Ban, & Christophe Boesch
(2009) Forest chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) remember the location of numerous fruit trees. Animal Cognition, 12 (6), 797–807. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Osvath, Mathias
(2009) Spontaneous planning for future stone throwing by a male chimpanzee. Current Biology, 19, R190–R191. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Osvath, Mathias & Helena Osvath
(2008) Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and orangutan (Pongo abelii) forethought: Self-control and pre-experience in the face of future tool use. Animal Cognition, 11, 661–674. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ouattara, Karim, Alban Lemasson, & Klaus Zuberbühler
(2009a) Campbell’s monkeys use affixation to alter call meaning. PLoS One, 4, e7808. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2009b) Campbell’s monkeys concatenate vocalizations into context-specific call sequences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106 (51), 22026–22031. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Owren, Michael J., R. Toby Amoss, & Drew Rendall
(2010) Two organizing principles of vocal production: Implications for nonhuman and human primates. American Journal of Primatology, 73, 530–544. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Patterson, Francine G.
(1978) The gestures of a gorilla: Language acquisition in another Pongid. Brain and Language, 5, 72–97. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pika, Simone, Katja Liebal, & Michael Tomasello
(2003) Gestural communication in young gorillas (Gorilla gorilla): Gestural repertoire, learning, and use. American Journal of Primatology, 60, 95–111. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2005) Gestural communication in subadult bonobos (Pan paniscus): Repertoire and use. American Journal of Primatology, 65, 39–61. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pika, Simone, Katja Liebal, Josep Call, & Michael Tomasello
(2005) The gestural communication of apes. In Katja Liebal, Cornelia Müller, & Simone Pika (Eds.), Gestural communication in nonhuman and human primates (pp. 37–51). Gesture, 5 (1/2). CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pika, Simone & John C. Mitani
(2008) The directed scratch: Evidence for a referential gesture in chimpanzees? In Rudolph Botha & Chris Knight (Eds.), The prehistory of language (pp. 166–180). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Russon, Anne E. & David R. Begun
(2004) Evolutionary origins of great ape intelligence: An integrated view. In Anne E. Russon & David R. Begun (Eds.), The evolution of thought (pp. 353–368). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Saussure, Ferdinand de
(2003) Cours de linguistique générale. Charles Bally & Albert Séchehaye (Eds.). Paris: Payot & Rivages (original work published 1916).Google Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. Sue, Kelly McDonald, Rose A. Sevcik, William D. Hopkins, & Elizabeth Rubert
(1986) Spontaneous symbol acquisition and communicative use by pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 211–235. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schel, Anne M., Simon W. Townsend, Zarin Machanda, Klaus Zuberbühler, & Katie E. Slocombe
(2013) Chimpanzee alarm call production meets key criteria for intentionality. PLoS ONE 01/2013; 8 (10), e76674. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schmelz, Martin, Josep Call, & Michael Tomasello
(2011) Chimpanzees know that others make inferences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 3077–3079. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Scott-Phillips, Thomas C.
(2015) Meaning in animal and human communication. Animal Cognition, 18 (3), 801–805. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Seyfarth, Robert M. & Dorothy L. Cheney
(2010) Production, usage, and comprehension in animal vocalizations. Brain & Language, 115, 92–100. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Slocombe, Katie E. & Klaus Zuberbühler
(2007) Chimpanzees modify recruitment screams as a function of audience composition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104 (43), 17228–17233. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Slocombe, Katie E., Tanja Kaller, Laurel Turman, Simon W. Townsend, Sarah Papworth, Paul Squibbs, & Klaus Zuberbühler
(2010) Production of food-associated calls in wild male chimpanzees is dependent on the composition of the audience. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 64 (12), 1959–1966. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sugiyama, Yukimaru
(1981) Observations on the population dynamics and behavior of wild chimpanzees of Bossou, Guinea, in 1979–1980. Primates, 22, 435–444. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, Michael, Deborah Gust, & G. Thomas Frost
(1989) A longitudinal investigation of gestural communication in young chimpanzees. Primates, 30, 35–50. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, Michael, Josep Call, & Brian Hare
(1998) Five primate species follow the visual gaze of conspecifics. Animal Behaviour, 55, 1063–1069. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, Michael, Brian Hare, & Bryan Agnetta
(1999) Chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, follow gaze direction geometrically. Animal Behaviour, 58, 769–777. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, Michael, Brian Hare, & Tara Fogleman
(2001) The ontogeny of gaze following in chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, and rhesus macaques, Macaca mulatta . Animal Behaviour, 61, 335–343. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, Michael, Josep Call, & Brian Hare
(2003) Chimpanzees understand psychological states – the question is which ones and to what extent. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 153–156. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
van Schaik, Carel P., Laura Damerius, & Karin Isler
(2013) Wild orangutan males plan and communicate their travel direction one day in advance. PLoS ONE, 8 (9), e74896. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Watts, David & John C. Mitani
(2001) Boundary patrols and intergroup encounters in wild chimpanzees. Behaviour, 138, 299–327. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Whiten, Andrew, Jane Goodall, William C. McGrew, Toshisada Nishida, Vernon Reynolds, Yukimaru Sugiyama, Caroline E.G. Tutin, Richard W. Wrangham, & Christophe Boesch
(1999) Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature, 399, 682–685. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2001) Charting cultural variation in chimpanzees. Behaviour, 138, 1481–1516. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wich, Serge A., Michael Krützen, Adriano R. Lameira, Alexander Nater, Natasha Arora, Meredith L. Bastian, Ellen Meulman, Helen C. Morrogh-Bernard, S. Suci Utami Atmoko, Joko Pamungkas, Dyah Perwitasari-Farajallah, Madelaine E. Hardus, Maria van Noordwijk, & Carel P. van Schaik
(2012) Call cultures in orang-utans? PLoS one, 7, e36180. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, Michael L., Marc D. Hauser, & Richard W. Wrangham
(2007) Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) modify grouping and vocal behaviour in response to location-specific risk. Behaviour, 144, 1621–1653. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Zuberbühler, Klaus
(2002) A syntactic rule in forest monkey communication. Animal Behaviour, 63, 293–299. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Zuberbühler, Klaus & Karline R.L. Janmaat
(2010) Foraging cognition in non-human primates. In Michael L. Platt & Asif A. Ghazanfar (Eds.), Primate neuroethology (pp. 64–83). Oxford: Oxford University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 2 other publications

Boesch, Christophe
2019.  In The Chimpanzees of the Taï Forest,  pp. 178 ff. Crossref logo
Gawne, Lauren, Chelsea Krajcik, Helene N. Andreassen, Andrea L. Berez-Kroeker & Barbara F. Kelly
2019. Data transparency and citation in the journal Gesture . Gesture 18:1  pp. 83 ff. Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 31 january 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.