Article published in:
Gesture
Vol. 15:1 (2016) ► pp. 6078
References

References

Barrett, Lisa F., Christine D. Wilson-Mendenhall, & Lawrence W. Barsalou
(2014) A psychological construction account of emotion regulation and dysregulation: The role of situated conceptualizations. In James J. Gross (Ed.), The handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 447–465). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Barsalou, Lawrence W.
(1999) Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 22, 577–660.Google Scholar
Barsalou, Lawrence W. & Katja Wiemer-Hastings
(2005) Situated abstract concepts. In Diane Pecher & Rolf A. Zwaan (Eds.), Grounding cognition: The role of perception and action in memory, language, and thinking (pp. 129–163). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bergen, Benjamin
(2012) Louder than words: The new science of how the mind makes meaning. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Bernardis, Paolo, Elena Salillas, & Nicoletta Caramelli
(2008) Behavioural and neurophysiological evidence of semantic interaction between iconic gestures and words. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25 (7/8), 1114–1128. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bloem, Ineke, Sylvia van den Boogaard, & Wido La Heij
(2004) Semantic facilitation and semantic interference in language production: Further evidence for the conceptual selection model of lexical access. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 307–323. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Damasio, Antonio
(1999) The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness. New York: Harcourt.Google Scholar
Diederich, Adele & Hans Colonius
(2015) The time window of multisensory integration: Relating reaction times and judgments of temporal order. Psychological Review 2015, Feb 23. Online at http://​www​.academia​.edu​/9450758​/The​_time​_window​_of​_multisensory​_integration​_relating​_reaction​_times​_and​_judgments​_of​_temporal​_order, last date of access 20 March 2015. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gellatly, Angus R.H. & Vernon H. Gregg
(1974) The effect of set size on the latency to name words. Acta Psychologica, 38, 93–98. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gentner, Dedre, Özyürek, Aslı, Özge Gürcanli, & Susan Goldin-Meadow
(2013) Spatial language facilitates spatial cognition: Evidence from children who lack language input. Cognition, 127, 318–330. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W.
(2003) Embodied experience and linguistic meaning. Brain & Language, 84, 1–15. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, James J.
(1979) The ecological approach to visual perception. Hillsday: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Glenberg, Arthur M. & Michael P. Kaschak
(2002) Grounding language in action. Psychonomic Bulleting & Review, 9, 558–565. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hauk, Olaf, Ingrid Johnsrude, & Friedemann Pulvermueller
(2004) Somatotopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex. Neuron, 41, 301–307. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Holle, Henning & Thomas C. Gunter
(2007) The role of iconic gestures in speech disambiguation: ERP evidence. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19 (7), 1175–1192. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hostetter, Autumn B. & Martha W. Alibali
(2008) Visible embodiment: Gestures as simulated action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15 (3), 495–514. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kelly, Spencer D., Aslı Özyürek, & Eric Maris
(2010) Two sides of the same coin: Speech and gesture mutually interact to enhance comprehension. Psychological Science, 21, 260–267. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kelly, Spencer D., Peter Creigh, & James Bartolotti
(2010) Integrating speech and iconic gestures in a Stroop-like task: Evidence for automatic processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 683–694. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, George & Mark Johnson
(1980) Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
(1999) Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
McNamara, Timothy P.
(2005) Semantic priming: Perspectives from memory and word recognition. New York: Psychology Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mayr, Susanne & Alex Buchner
(2007) Negative priming as a memory phenomenon. A review of 20 years of negative priming research. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 215 (1), 35–51. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Metta, Giorgio, et al.
(2008) The iCub humanoid robot: An open platform for research in embodied cognition. In Raj Madhavan & Elena R. Messina (Eds.), Proceedings of the 8th Workshop on Performance Metrics for Intelligent Systems (pp. 50–56). New York: ACM. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Neely, James H.
(1991) Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories. In Derek Besner & Glyn W. Humphreys (Eds.), Basic processes in reading: Visual word recognition (pp. 264–336). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
Neggers, Bas & Harold Bekkering
(2001) Gaze anchoring to a pointing target is present during the entire pointing movement and is driven by a non-visual signal. Journal of Neurophysiology, 86, 961–970.Google Scholar
Niebur, Ernst, et al.
(2001) Research, robots, and reality: A statement on current trends in biorobotics. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24 (6), 1072–1073. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Obermeier, Christian & Thomas C. Gunter
(2014) Multisensory integration: The case of a time window of gesture-speech integration. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27 (2), 292–307. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pouw, Wim T.J.L.. et al.
(2014) Toward a more embedded/extended perspective on the cognitive function of gestures. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 359. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Prinz, Jesse J. & Lawrence W. Barsalou
(2000) Steering a course for embodied representation. In Eric Dietrich & Arthur B. Markman (Eds.), Cognitive dynamics: Conceptual and representational change in humans and machines (pp. 51–77). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, Giacomo & Laila Craighero
(2004) The mirror neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rizzolatti, Giacomo, Luciano Fadiga, Vittorio Gallese, & Leonardo Fogassi
(1996) Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Cognitive Brain Research, 3, 131–141. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Seliger, Herbert
(1980) Utterance planning and correction behavior: Its function in the grammar construction process for second language learners. In Hans W. Dechert & Manfred Raupach (Eds.), Towards a crosslinguistic assessment of speech production (pp. 87–99). Frankfurt am Main, Germany: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
So, Wing-Chee, Alvan Low Yi-Feng, De-Fu Yap, Eugene Kheng, & Ju-Min Melvin Yap
(2013) Iconic gestures prime words: Comparison of priming effects when gestures are presented alone and when they are accompanying speech. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 779. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tipper, Steven P.
(1985) The negative priming effect: Inhibitory priming by ignored objects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 37 (4), 571–590. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, Barbara & Brigitte M. Hard
(2009) Embodied and disembodied cognition: Spatial perspective taking. Cognition, 110, 124–129. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Vacharkulksemsuk, Tanya & Barbara L. Fredrickson
(2012) Strangers in sync: Achieving embodied rapport through shared movements. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 399–402. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Vainiger, Dana, Ludovica Labruna, Richard Ivry, & Michal Lavidor
(2014) Beyond words: Evidence for automatic language–gesture integration of symbolic gestures but not dynamic landscapes. Psychological Research, 78, 55–69. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
van Dantzig, Saskia, René Zeelenberg, & Diane Pecher
(2009) Unconstraining theories of embodied cognition. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 345–351. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Yap, De-Fu, Wing-Chee So, Yu-Min Melvin Yap, Tan, Ying-Quan, & Ruo-Li Serene Teoh
(2011) Iconic gestures prime words. Cognitive Science, 35 (1), 171–183. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ziemke, Tom & Robert Lowe
(2009) On the role of emotion in embodied cognitive architectures: From organisms to robots. Cognitive Computation, 1 (1), 104–117. CrossrefGoogle Scholar