Article published in:
The Constitution of Phenomenal Consciousness: Toward a science and theory
Edited by Steven M. Miller
[Advances in Consciousness Research 92] 2015
► pp. 1747
Cited by

Cited by 3 other publications

Davidson, Matthew J, Will Mithen, Hinze Hogendoorn, Jeroen JA van Boxtel & Naotsugu Tsuchiya
2020. The SSVEP tracks attention, not consciousness, during perceptual filling-in. eLife 9 Crossref logo
Mateos, D. M., R. Guevara Erra, R. Wennberg & J. L. Perez Velazquez
2018. Measures of entropy and complexity in altered states of consciousness. Cognitive Neurodynamics 12:1  pp. 73 ff. Crossref logo
You, Yihong, Yiming Ma, Zhiguang Ji, Fanying Meng, Anmin Li & Chunhua Zhang
2018. Unconscious response inhibition differences between table tennis athletes and non-athletes. PeerJ 6  pp. e5548 ff. Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 03 april 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.

References
Arnold, D.H., Clifford, C.W., & Wenderoth, P.
(2001) Asynchronous processing in vision: Color leads motion. Current Biology, 11(8), 596–600. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Azzopardi, P., & Cowey, A.
(1997) Is blindsight like normal, near-threshold vision? Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 94(25), 14190–14194. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baars, B.J.
(1993) A cognitive theory of consciousness. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
(1997) In the theater of consciousness: The workspace of the mind. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Blake, R., & Logothetis, N.K.
(2002) Visual competition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(1), 13–21. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Block, N.
(1995) On a confusion about a function of consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18(2), 227–287. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1996) How can we find the neural correlate of consciousness? Trends in Neurosciences, 19(11), 456–459. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2005a) Two neural correlates of consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(2), 46–52. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2005b) The merely verbal problem of consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(6), 270. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2011) Perceptual consciousness overflows cognitive access. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(12), 567–575. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bonneh, Y.S., Cooperman, A., & Sagi, D.
(2001) Motion-induced blindness in normal observers. Nature, 411(6839), 798–801. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Born, R.T., & Bradley, D.C.
(2005) Structure and function of visual area MT. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 28, 157–189. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bosman, C.A., Schoffelen, J.-M., Brunet, N., Oostenveld, R., Bastos, A.M., Womelsdorf, T., Rubehn, B., Stieglitz, T., De Weerd, P., & Fries, P.
(2012) Attentional stimulus selection through selective synchronization between monkey visual areas. Neuron, 75(5), 875–888. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Braun, A.R., Balkin, T.J., Wesensten, N.J., Gwadry, F., Carson, R.E., Varga, M., Baldwin, P., Belenky, G., & Herscovitch, P.
(1998) Dissociated pattern of activity in visual cortices and their projections during human rapid eye movement sleep. Science, 279(5347), 91–95. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Broadbent, D.E., & Broadbent, M.H.
(1987) From detection to identification: Response to multiple targets in rapid serial visual presentation. Perception & Psychophysics, 42(2), 105–113. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Carrasco, M., Ling, S., & Read, S.
(2004) Attention alters appearance. Nature Neuroscience, 7(3), 308–313. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chalmers, D.J.
(1995) Facing up to the problem of consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 2(3), 200–219.Google Scholar
(1996) The conscious mind: In search of a fundamental theory. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cohen, M.A., Cavanagh, P., Chun, M.M., & Nakayama, K.
(2012) The attentional requirements of consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(8), 411–417. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, M.A., & Dennett, D.C.
(2011) Consciousness cannot be separated from function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(8), 358–364. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Crick, F., & Koch, C.
(1990) Towards a neurobiological theory of consciousness. Seminars in the Neurosciences, 2, 263–275. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1995) Are we aware of neural activity in primary visual cortex? Nature, 375(6527), 121–123. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1998) Consciousness and neuroscience. Cerebral Cortex, 8(2), 97–107. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2003) A framework for consciousness. Nature Neuroscience, 6(2), 119–126. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dehaene, S., Changeux, J.-P., Naccache, L., Sackur, J., & Sergent, C.
(2006) Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: A testable taxonomy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(5), 204–211. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dehaene, S., Kerszberg, M., & Changeux, J.-P.
(1998) A neuronal model of a global workspace in effortful cognitive tasks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 95(24), 14529–14534. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dehaene, S., & Naccache, L.
(2001) Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: Basic evidence and a workspace framework. Cognition, 79(1–2), 1–37. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Cohen, L., Bihan, D.L., Mangin, J.F., Poline, J.B., & Rivière, D.
(2001) Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious repetition priming. Nature Neuroscience, 4(7), 752–758. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Le Clec’H, G., Koechlin, E., Mueller, M., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., van de Moortele, P.F., & Le Bihan, D.
(1998) Imaging unconscious semantic priming. Nature, 395(6702), 597–600. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Driver, J., & Mattingley, J.B.
(1998) Parietal neglect and visual awareness. Nature Neuroscience, 1(1), 17–22. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Edelman, G.M.
(1987) Neural darwinism: The theory of neuronal group selection. New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
(1993) Neural darwinism: Selection and reentrant signaling in higher brain function. Neuron, 10(2), 115–125. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Edelman, G.M., Gally, J.A., & Baars, B.J.
(2011) Biology of consciousness. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 4. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Edelman, G.M., & Mountcastle, V.B.
(1978) The mindful brain: Cortical organization and the group-selective theory of higher brain function. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Edelman, G.M., & Tononi, G.
(2000) A universe of consciousness: How matter becomes imagination. New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Fahrenfort, J.J., & Lamme, V.A.F.
(2012) A true science of consciousness explains phenomenology: Comment on Cohen and Dennett. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(3), 138–139. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fahrenfort, J.J., Scholte, H.S., & Lamme, V.A.F.
(2007) Masking disrupts reentrant processing in human visual cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(9), 1488–1497. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Felleman, D., & Van Essen, D.C.
(1991) Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 1(1), 1–47. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ferrera, V.P., Rudolph, K.K., & Maunsell, J.H.
(1994) Responses of neurons in the parietal and temporal visual pathways during a motion task. Journal of Neuroscience, 14(10), 6171–6186.Google Scholar
Gaillard, R., Dehaene, S., Adam, C., Clémenceau, S., Hasboun, D., Baulac, M., Cohen, L., & Nacchache, L.
(2009) Converging intracranial markers of conscious access. PLoS Biology, 7(3), e61. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Galvin, S.J., Podd, J.V, Drga, V., & Whitmore, J.
(2003) Type 2 tasks in the theory of signal detectability: Discrimination between correct and incorrect decisions. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(4), 843–876. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Goodale, M.A.
(2004) An evolving view of duplex vision: Separate but interacting cortical pathways for perception and action. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14(2), 203–211. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Goodale, M.A., & Milner, A.
(1992) Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends in Neurosciences, 15(1), 20–25. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Goodale, M.A., Milner, A.D., Jakobson, L.S., & Carey, D.P.
(1991) A neurological dissociation between perceiving objects and grasping them. Nature, 349(6305), 154–156. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Greenwald, A.G., Draine, S.C., & Abrams, R.L.
(1996) Three cognitive markers of unconscious semantic activation. Science, 273(5282), 1699–1702. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grill-Spector, K., & Malach, R.
(2004) The human visual cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 649–677. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hameroff, S.
(2006a) The entwined mysteries of anesthesia and consciousness: Is there a common underlying mechanism? Anesthesiology, 105(2), 400–412. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2006b) Consciousness, neurobiology and quantum mechanics: The case for a connection. In J.A. Tuszynski (Ed.), The emerging physics of consciousness (pp.193–253). Berlin: Springer-Verlag. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hopfinger, J.B., Buonocore, M.H., & Mangun, G.R.
(2000) The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control. Nature Neuroscience, 3(3), 284–291. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Horikawa, T., Tamaki, M., Miyawaki, Y., & Kamitani, Y.
(2013) Neural decoding of visual imagery during sleep. Science, 340(6132), 639–642. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Intraub, H.
(1997) The representation of visual scenes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1(6), 217–222. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jackendoff, R.
(1996) How language helps us think. Pragmatics & Cognition, 4(1), 1–34. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
James, T.W., Culham, J., Humphrey, G.K., Milner, A.D., & Goodale, M.A.
(2003) Ventral occipital lesions impair object recognition but not object-directed grasping: An fMRI study. Brain, 126(11), 2463–2475. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kim, C., & Blake, R.
(2005) Psychophysical magic: Rendering the visible “invisible”. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(8), 381–388. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Klein, I., Paradis, A.-L., Poline, J.B., Kosslyn, S.M., & Le Bihan, D.
(2000) Transient activity in the human calcarine cortex during visual-mental imagery: An event-related fMRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(Suppl. 2), 15–23. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Knauff, M., Kassubek, J., Mulack, T., & Greenlee, M.W.
(2000) Cortical activation evoked by visual mental imagery as measured by fMRI. Neuroreport, 11(18), 3957–3962. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ko, Y., & Lau, H.
(2012) A detection theoretic explanation of blindsight suggests a link between conscious perception and metacognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 367(1594), 1401–1411. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Koch, C.
(2004) The quest for consciousness: A neurobiological approach. Englewood, CO: Roberts & Company.Google Scholar
Koch, C., & Hepp, K.
(2006) Quantum mechanics in the brain. Nature, 440(7084), 611. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Koch, C., & Tsuchiya, N.
(2007) Attention and consciousness: Two distinct brain processes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(1), 16–22. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2012) Attention and consciousness: Related yet different. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(2), 103–104. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kolb, F.C., & Braun, J.
(1995) Blindsight in normal observers. Nature, 377(6547), 336–338. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kornbrot, D.E.
(2006) Signal detection theory, the approach of choice: Model-based and distribution-free measures and evaluation. Perception & Psychophysics, 68(3), 393–414. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kosslyn, S.M., Thompson, W.L., Kim, I.J., & Alpert, N.M.
(1995) Topographical representations of mental images in primary visual cortex. Nature, 378(6556), 496–498. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kouider, S., De Gardelle, V., Sackur, J., & Dupoux, E.
(2010) How rich is consciousness? The partial awareness hypothesis. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(7), 301–307. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kouider, S., & Dehaene, S.
(2007) Levels of processing during non-conscious perception: A critical review of visual masking. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 362(1481), 857–875. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kunimoto, C., Miller, J., & Pashler, H.
(2001) Confidence and accuracy of near-threshold discrimination responses. Consciousness and Cognition, 10(3), 294–340. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lamme, V.A.F.
(1995) The neurophysiology of figure-ground segregation in primary visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 15(2), 1605–1615.Google Scholar
(2003) Why visual attention and awareness are different. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(1), 12–18. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2004) Separate neural definitions of visual consciousness and visual attention: A case for phenomenal awareness. Neural Networks, 17(5–6), 861–872. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2006) Towards a true neural stance on consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(11), 494–501. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2010) How neuroscience will change our view on consciousness. Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(3), 204–220. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lamme, V.A.F., & Roelfsema, P.R.
(2000) The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing. Trends in Neurosciences, 23(11), 571–579. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lamme, V.A.F., Supèr, H., & Spekreijse, H.
(1998) Feedforward, horizontal, and feedback processing in the visual cortex. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 8(4), 529–535. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lamme, V.A.F., Zipser, K., & Spekreijse, H.
(2002) Masking interrupts figure-ground signals in V1. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(7), 1044–1053. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Landman, R., Spekreijse, H., & Lamme, V.A.F.
(2003) Large capacity storage of integrated objects before change blindness. Vision Research, 43(2), 149–164. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Levi, D.M.
(2008) Crowding – An essential bottleneck for object recognition: A mini-review. Vision Research, 48(5), 635–654. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Levine, J.
(1983) Materialism and qualia: The explanatory gap. Pacific Philosophical Quarterly, 64(4), 354–361.Google Scholar
Luck, S.J., & Vogel, E.K.
(1997) The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions. Nature, 390(6657), 279–281. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mack, A.
(2003) Inattentional blindness: Looking without seeing. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12(5), 180–184. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mack, A., & Rock, I.
(1998) Inattentional blindness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Macknik, S.L.
(2006) Visual masking approaches to visual awareness. Progress in Brain Research, 155, 177–215. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Macmillan, N.A., & Creelman, D.C.
(1991) Detection theory: A user’s guide. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Maniscalco, B., & Lau, H.
(2012) A signal detection theoretic approach for estimating metacognitive sensitivity from confidence ratings. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(1), 422–430. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Marcel, A.J.
(1983) Conscious and unconscious perception: Experiments on visual masking and word recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 15(2), 197–237. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Marchetti, G.
(2012) Against the view that consciousness and attention are fully dissociable. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 36. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Martens, S., & Wyble, B.
(2010) The attentional blink: Past, present, and future of a blind spot in perceptual awareness. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(6), 947–957. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Maunsell, J.H.R., & Newsome, W.T.
(1987) Visual processing in monkey extrastriate cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 10, 363–401. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
McFadden, J.
(2002a) Synchronous firing and its influence on the brains electromagnetic field. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 9(4), 23–50.Google Scholar
(2002b) The conscious electromagnetic information (Cemi) field theory: The hard problem made easy? Journal of Consciousness Studies, 9(8), 45–60.Google Scholar
Merigan, W.H., & Maunsell, J.H.
(1993) How parallel are the primate visual pathways? Annual Review of Neuroscience, 16, 369–402. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Merikle, P., Smilek, D., & Eastwood, J.
(2001) Perception without awareness: Perspectives from cognitive psychology. Cognition, 79(1–2), 115–134. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mesulam, M.M.
(1998) From sensation to cognition. Brain, 121(6), 1013–1052. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Miller, S.M.
(2001) Binocular rivalry and the cerebral hemispheres: With a note on the correlates and constitution of visual consciousness. Brain and Mind, 2(1), 119–149. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2007) On the correlation/constitution distinction problem (and other hard problems) in the scientific study of consciousness. Acta Neuropsychiatrica, 19(3), 159–176. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(Ed.) (2013) The constitution of visual consciousness: Lessons from binocular rivalry. Advances in Consciousness Research (Vol. 90). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins Publishing Company. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Milner, A., & Goodale, M.A.
(1993) Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Progress in Brain Research, 95, 317–337. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mishkin, M., & Ungerleider, L.G.
(1982) Contribution of striate inputs to the visuospatial functions of parieto-preoccipital cortex in monkeys. Behavioural Brain Research, 6(1), 57–77. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Moutoussis, K., & Zeki, S.
(1997a) A direct demonstration of perceptual asynchrony in vision. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 264(1380), 393–399. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1997b) Functional segregation and temporal hierarchy of the visual perceptive systems. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 264(1387), 1407–1414. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2002) The relationship between cortical activation and perception investigated with invisible stimuli. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 99(14), 9527–9532. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nagel, T.
(1974) What is it like to be a bat? Philosophical Review, 83(4), 435–450. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nakamura, K., Dehaene, S., Jobert, A., Le Bihan, D., & Kouider, S.
(2007) Task-specific change of unconscious neural priming in the cerebral language network. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 104(49), 19643–19648. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Oizumi, M., Albantakis, L., & Tononi, G.
(2014) From the phenomenology to the mechanisms of consciousness: Integrated Information Theory 3.0. PLoS Computational Biology, 10(5), e1003588. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
O’Regan, J.K., Rensink, R.A., & Clark, J.J.
(1999) Change-blindness as a result of “mudsplashes”. Nature, 398(6722), 34. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Owen, A.M., & Coleman, M.R.
(2008) Detecting awareness in the vegetative state. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1129, 130–138. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Owen, A.M., Coleman, M.R., Boly, M., Davis, M.H., Laureys, S., & Pickard, J.D.
(2006) Detecting awareness in the vegetative state. Science, 313(5792), 1402. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pelli, D.G., Palomares, M., & Majaj, N.J.
(2004) Crowding is unlike ordinary masking: Distinguishing feature integration from detection. Journal of Vision, 4(12), 12. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pelli, D.G., & Tillman, K.A.
(2008) The uncrowded window of object recognition. Nature Neuroscience, 11(10), 1129–1135. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Persaud, N., McLeod, P., & Cowey, A.
(2007) Post-decision wagering objectively measures awareness. Nature Neuroscience, 10(2), 257–261. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pinto, Y., Sligte, I.G., Shapiro, K.L., & Lamme, V.A.F.
(2013) Fragile visual short-term memory is an object-based and location-specific store. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(4), 732–739. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pöppel, E., Brinkmann, R., Von Cramon, D., & Singer, W.
(1978) Association and dissociation of visual functions in a case of bilateral occipital lobe infarction. Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten, 225(1), 1–21. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pöppel, E., Held, R., & Frost, D.
(1973) Residual visual function after brain wounds involving the central visual pathways in man. Nature, 243(5405), 295–296. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Posner, M.I.
(1994) Attention: The mechanisms of consciousness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 91(16), 7398–7403. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2012) Attentional networks and consciousness. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 64. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Potter, M.C., Chun, M.M., Banks, B.S., & Muckenhoupt, M.
(1998) Two attentional deficits in serial target search: The visual attentional blink and an amodal task-switch deficit. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24(4), 979–992. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Price, N.S.C.
(2013) Overview of visual system structure and function. In S.M. Miller (Ed.), The constitution of visual consciousness: Lessons from binocular rivalry (pp.37–76). Advances in Consciousness Research (Vol. 90). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins Publishing Company. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Raffone, A., & Pantani, M.
(2010) A global workspace model for phenomenal and access consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition, 19(2), 580–596. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Raymond, J.E., Shapiro, K.L., & Arnell, K.M.
(1992) Temporary suppression of visual processing in an RSVP task: An attentional blink? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18(3), 849–860. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rees, G., Kreiman, G., & Koch, C.
(2002) Neural correlates of consciousness in humans. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(4), 261–270. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rees, G., Wojciulik, E., Clarke, K., Husain, M., Frith, C., & Driver, J.
(2000) Unconscious activation of visual cortex in the damaged right hemisphere of a parietal patient with extinction. Brain, 123(8), 1624–1633. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2002) Neural correlates of conscious and unconscious vision in parietal extinction. Neurocase, 8(5), 387–393. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rensink, R.A., O’Regan, J.K., & Clark, J.J.
(1997) To see or not to see: The need for attention to perceive changes in scenes. Psychological Science, 8(5), 368–373. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Revonsuo, A.
(2001) Can functional brain imaging discover consciousness in the brain? Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8(3), 3–23.Google Scholar
Roe, A.W., Chelazzi, L., Connor, C.E., Conway, B.R., Fujita, I., Gallant, J.L., Lu, H., & Vanduffel, W.
(2012) Toward a unified theory of visual area V4. Neuron, 74(1), 12–29. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Roelfsema, P.R.
(2006) Cortical algorithms for perceptual grouping. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 29, 203–227. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Seidemann, E., Poirson, A.B., Wandell, B.A., & Newsome, W.T.
(1999) Color signals in area MT of the macaque monkey. Neuron, 24(4), 911–917. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Seth, A.K.
(2009) Explanatory correlates of consciousness: Theoretical and computational challenges. Cognitive Computation, 1(1), 50–63. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2010) The grand challenge of consciousness. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 5. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Simons, D.J., & Chabris, C.F.
(1999) Gorillas in our midst: Sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 28(9), 1059–1074. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Simons, D.J., & Levin, D.T.
(1997) Change blindness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1(7), 261–267. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Simons, D.J., & Rensink, R.A.
(2005) Change blindness: Past, present, and future. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(1), 16–20. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sligte, I.G., Scholte, H.S., & Lamme, V.A.F.
(2008) Are there multiple visual short-term memory stores? PLoS ONE, 3(2), e1699. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2009) V4 activity predicts the strength of visual short-term memory representations. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(23), 7432–7438. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sligte, I.G., Wokke, M.E., Tesselaar, J.P., Scholte, H.S., & Lamme, V.A.F.
(2010) Magnetic stimulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex dissociates fragile visual short-term memory from visual working memory. Neuropsychologia, 49(6), 1578–1588. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sperling, G.
(1960) Negative afterimage without prior positive image. Science, 131(3413), 1613–1614. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stoerig, P., & Cowey, A.
(2007) Blindsight. Current Biology, 17(19), R822–R824. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tononi, G.
(2004) An information integration theory of consciousness. BMC Neuroscience, 5, 42. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2008) Consciousness as integrated information: A provisional manifesto. Biological Bulletin, 215(3), 216–242. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tononi, G., & Edelman, G.M.
(1998) Consciousness and complexity. Science, 282(5395), 1846–1851. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tononi, G., & Koch, C.
(2008) The neural correlates of consciousness: An update. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 239–261. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tononi, G., & Sporns, O.
(2003) Measuring information integration. BMC Neuroscience, 4, 31. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tovee, M.J.
(1994) How fast is the speed of thought? Current Biology, 4(12), 1125–1127. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Treue, S.
(2003) Visual attention: The where, what, how and why of saliency. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13(4), 428–432. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tsuchiya, N., & Koch, C.
(2005) Continuous flash suppression reduces negative afterimages. Nature Neuroscience, 8(8), 1096–1101. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
van Boxtel, J.J.A., Tsuchiya, N., & Koch, C.
(2010a) Consciousness and attention: On sufficiency and necessity. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 217. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2010b) Opposing effects of attention and consciousness on afterimages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 107(19), 8883–8888. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Vandenbroucke, A.R., Sligte, I.G., & Lamme, V.A.F.
(2011) Manipulations of attention dissociate fragile visual short-term memory from visual working memory. Neuropsychologia, 49(6), 1559–1568. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wallis, T.S.A., & Bex, P.J.
(2011) Visual crowding is correlated with awareness. Current Biology, 21(3), 254–258. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Watanabe, M., Cheng, K., Murayama, Y., Ueno, K., Asamizuya, T., Tanaka, K., & Logothetis, N.
(2011) Attention but not awareness modulates the BOLD signal in the human V1 during binocular suppression. Science, 334(6057), 829–831. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Weiskrantz, L., Barbur, J.L., & Sahraie, A.
(1995) Parameters affecting conscious versus unconscious visual discrimination with damage to the visual cortex (V1). Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 92(13), 6122–6126. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Weiskrantz, L., Warrington, E.K., Sanders, M.D., & Marshall, J.
(1974) Visual capacity in the hemianopic field following a restricted occipital ablation. Brain, 97(4), 709–728. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Whitney, D., & Levi, D.M.
(2011) Visual crowding: A fundamental limit on conscious perception and object recognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(4), 160–168. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wilke, M., Logothetis, N.K., & Leopold, D.A.
(2003) Generalized flash suppression of salient visual targets. Neuron, 39(6), 1043–1052. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wolfe, J.M.
(1984) Reversing ocular dominance and suppression in a single flash. Vision Research, 24(5), 471–478. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Zeki, S.
(2001) Localization and globalization in conscious vision. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 57–86. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2008) The disunity of consciousness. Progress in Brain Research, 168, 11–18. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Zeki, S., & Bartels, A.
(1998a) The autonomy of the visual systems and the modularity of conscious vision. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 353(1377), 1911–1914. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1998b) The asynchrony of consciousness. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 265(1405), 1583–1585. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Zeki, S., & ffytche, D.H.
(1998) The Riddoch syndrome: Insights into the neurobiology of conscious vision. Brain, 121(1), 25–45. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Zipser, K., Lamme, V.A.F., & Schiller, P.H.
(1996) Contextual modulation in primary visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 16(22), 7376–7389.Google Scholar